Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Behavioral-psychological motivations encoded in the vocal repertoire of captive Amur tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) cubs

Abstract

Background

The Amur tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) is the largest and one of the most endangered cats in the world. In wild and captive cats, communication is mainly dependent on olfaction. However, vocal communication also plays a key role between mother and cubs during the breeding period. How cubs express their physiological and psychological needs to their mother and companions by using acoustic signals is little known and mainly hindered by the difficult process of data collection. Here, we quantitatively summarized the vocal repertoire and behavioral contexts of captive Amur tiger cubs. The aim of the present work was to investigate the behavioral motivations of cub calls by considering influential factors of age, sex, and rearing experiences.

Results

The 5335 high-quality calls from 65 tiger cubs were classified into nine call types (Ar-1, Ar-2, Er, eee, Chuff, Growl, Hiss, Haer, and Roar) produced in seven behavioral contexts. Except for Er, eight of the nine call types were context-specific, related to Play (Ar-2, eee, and Roar), Isolation (Ar-1), Offensive Context (Haer, Growl, and Hiss), and a friendly context (Chuff).

Conclusions

The results suggest that cubs are not quiet, but instead they express rich information by emitting various call types, which are probably crucial for survival in the wild. We herein provide the first detailed spectrogram classification to indicate vocal repertoires of calls and their coding with respect to behavioral contexts in Amur tiger cubs, and we pave the steps for revealing their social communication system, which can be applied for conservation of populations. These insights can help tiger managers or keepers to improve the rearing conditions by understanding the feline cubs’ inner status and needs by monitoring their vocal information expressions and exchanges.

Background

Vocal signals are the essential form of communication in animals [1,2,3]. Animals use vocal communication to maintain social relations, including mate selection and intercourse, competition, and signal food and security. Understanding the critical role of vocalization in animal communication systems can be an essential tool for the conservation of endangered species [4]. In altricial species, infant calls can trigger adaptive care-giving behaviors from parents, which can even determine infant survival [5,6,7]. The acoustic parameters of kitten calls depict their emotional state and maintain their individuality [8, 9]. Some studies have indicated that non-human adult animals respond to infant calls of various mammalian species [5, 10]. In some species of, for instance, birds [11, 12], felids [13], and rodents, [14], infants use acoustic signals to compete in situations of limited resources.

The Amur tiger (Panthera tigris altaica), also known as the Siberian tiger, is the largest felid in the world [15]. This attractive species inhabits threatened environments in the wild, and it has been listed in the International Union for Conservation of Nature’s (IUCN) Red List as Endangered [16]. All tiger subspecies are solitary [17], which makes it is nearly impossible to locate and directly observe their behavior in the wild. In China, Amur tigers have been successfully bred in captivity for more than 30 years [18], with a large captive population of about 1000 individuals in Amur Tiger Parks.

Few studies have focused on basic postures, habits, and behaviors of captive Amur tigers [19,20,21,22,23]. Regarding Amur tiger vocalization, very limited information is available [24], mainly characterizing these animals’ prusten, growl, snarl, grunt, moan, meow, spit, hiss, and roar [24]. Information about vocal repertorie [25], individual signatures [26], habitat influence on call frequency [27], and physiology base of vocalization production [28,29,30,31] have been investigated in other Felidae species. Acoustic research on tiger cubs is also very limited. These researches focused on factors correlated with vocal development of felid calls, including body size and vocal tract length [32]. The most studied felid species regarding infant vocalization is the domestic cat (Felis catus) [33, 34] and the call parameters of kittens showed developmental changes, and motivational valuation of the rearing situations [35]. Regarding large cats, the vocal repertoire of calls of adult and cub cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) has been systematically described [36, 37]. Calls of cubs of many other species have also been shown to have developmental dynamics during ontogeny [38,39,40,41,42,43,44].

Exploring the acoustic structure and developmental dynamics of tiger cub vocalizations can provide a foundation for uncovering the communication system of tigers, especially regarding mother-offspring interaction. We hypothesized that there would be a quantitative relationship between the vocal repertoire and behavioral context of calls of captive Amur tiger cubs.

Results

A total of 5335 samples from 65 tiger cubs were extracted for analysis. The distribution of calls differed by rearing condition and sex (Table 1).

Table 1 Sample size distribution in different rearing conditions and sexes

We identified eight distinct call types: Ar (n = 3679), Er (n = 628), Eee (n = 105), Chuff (n = 428), Growl (n = 67), Hiss (n = 211), Haer (n = 126), and Roar (n = 91). In this study, Growl was not recorded for hand-reared cubs. Not all 11 acoustic parameters were measured for each call type. For non-laryngeal sounds (i.e., Hiss), we did not measure the fundamental-frequency-related parameters.

Vocal repertoire of Amur Tiger cubs

The PCA extracted four principal components (PC1–4), accumulatively explaining 87% of the sample variance, with PC5–7 capturing another 10% of the sample variance. The highest loading factors of the first four PCs were explicit. PC1 was driven by MaxFreq, Freq25, Freq50, and Freq75. PC2 showed a high correlation of temporal parameters (duration and risk time). PC3 was heavily influenced by three fundamental frequency parameters (F0, MaxF0, and MinF0). PC4 was dominated by pulse parameters (PulseNum and PulseRate, Table 2).

Table 2 Proportion of variance of the first four principal components, with acoustic parameters ranked by factor loadings

The results of the hierarchical cluster analysis showed that the samples were distinctly clustered by three calls with rhythmic pulsation (eee, Growl, and Chuff) and Hiss (Fig. 1). However, the calls of eee and Growl were clustered together. The samples of Ar were separated into two groups. One was clearly clustered separately from another sample, and the other was assigned with nearly all samples of three call types (Er, Haer, and Roar) (Fig. 1). Based on this classification, we labeled the groups as Ar-1 and Ar-2. To identify if Ar-1 and Ar-2 were different call types, we compared each of the 11 parameters for these two call types. Non-parametric tests showed that all acoustic parameters significantly differed between these groups (Mann-Whitney U-test: U = 85,513–1,556,825, p < 0.001). Therefore, we adopted Ar-1 and Ar-2 as a replacement for Ar, and the complete samples were reclassified into nine call types.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Dendrogram from hierarchical cluster analysis using PC scores. Priori call types (Ar, Chuff, eee, Er, Growl, Haer, Hiss, Roar) of each sample were shown below. The samples of Ar were labeled by two classifications (Ar-1 and Ar-2)

Four discriminant functions from PC1–4 revealed significant differences between the nine call types. The first two discriminant functions collectively explained 98.3% of the variance. Among them, the first discriminant function (DF1) explained 77.7% of the data and had a higher discriminating ability among the nine call types of the Amur tiger cubs. Of the 5335 experimental calls, 3834 calls were correctly classified, indicating that the percentage of correct classification was 71.9% (cross-validation = 71.8%), which is better than the percent correct discrimination attributed to chance (11.1%, binomial test, p < 0.001). Among the correctly classified calls, the correct classification rates of Chuff, Hiss, and Ar-1 were high (100, 98.6, and 94.6%, respectively). The correct classification rate of Er was low (34.1%), with most of the samples having been assigned to Roar and Ar-2. The lowest correct classification rate was 23% (pertaining to Haer), with most samples having been assigned to Er and Roar (see Additional file 2).

The call types that were classified together in the cluster analysis also had overlaps in the result of DFA (eee and Growl; Ar-2, Er, Haer, and Roar). To identify the acoustic parameters that differed between them, we used a non-parametric test to compare 11 or 13 of their parameters. All 11 parameters between Ar-2, Er, Haer, and Roar were significantly different (Kruskal-Wallis test: χ2 = 138.857–855.801, df = 3, p < 0.001). Ten of the 13 parameters for eee and Growl were significantly different (Mann-Whitney U-test: U = 598.5–3479, p < 0.001–0.895): MaxFreq, F0, MaxF0, MinF0, Freq25, Freq50, Freq75, IQRBW, PulseNum, and PulseRate. From this analysis, nine call types produced by the Amur tiger cubs were identified (Fig. 2A–I; Table 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Spectrograms of nine call types of captive Amur tiger cubs. Spectrograms of Ar-1, Ar-2, Chuff, Growl, Er, eee, Haer, Hiss, and Roar are shown in (A)–(I), separately. The spectrograms were created at a FFT size of 2048, Hann window of 1024 samples, and overlap of 50%

Table 3 Values (mean ± SD) of acoustic parameters for the call types of Amur tiger cubs. The total number of each call type is given in the parentheses

Behavioral context

We initially preset six behavioral contexts in which calls were produced; however, we later added a new behavioral context named “Suckled”, resulting in a total of seven behavioral contexts. Suckled context refers to the sucking behavior given by one cub to the other, usually at the ear. We found that all calls in the Suckled and Play context were uttered from cubs who were sucked, bitten or pressed by the other, to express uncomfortable feelings.

Eight of nine call types were context-specific (i.e., given mostly in one specific context), including Ar-1 (Isolation), Ar-2 (Play), Chuff (Human-Contact), eee (Play), Growl (Offensive-to-Human), Haer (Offensive-to-Conspecific), Hiss (Offensive-to-Human), and Roar (Play; Table 4). Except for Ar-2 and Roar, the other six context-specific call types were given in only one context in more than 80% of cases.

Table 4 Context-specificity calls of Amur tiger cubs

Six of the seven contexts have a unique signal-specific call type (i.e., the most common call type used in a specific context), including Isolation (Ar-1), Play (Ar-2), Human-Contact and Conspecific-Contact (Chuff), Offensive-to-Conspecific (Haer), and Offensive-to-Human (Hiss; Table 5). A total of five signal-specific call types were also context-specific call types. Chuff was a signal-specific call type for two contexts. The remaining three context-specific call types (eee, Growl, and Roar) were not the only call types given during a specific behavioral context.

Table 5 Signal-specificity call of Amur tiger cubs

Ar-1 is the context-specific call type and signal-specific call type of the isolation context, and it is similar to the isolation call of domestic kittens [8, 33]. Based on previous data from the literature [33, 34], we regarded Ar-1 during the isolation context as the isolation call of Amur tiger cubs.

Discussion

Vocal repertoire of Amur Tiger cubs

In this study, nine call types produced by captive Amur tiger cubs were identified. The number of calls of each of the nine call types was not equal. The numbers of Ar-1 and Ar-2 were higher than those of other call types, comprising 32.3 and 36.6% of the whole sample dataset, respectively. This is the result of random sampling and the behavioral rhythm of Amur tiger cubs. Because of the experimental environment (breeding beds) and the motor and sensory capabilities of tiger cubs under two months old, calls mostly occurred in an isolation context. According to Yu’s study [45], except for sleep and rest, play is the main routine behavior of Amur tigers after two months of age (play accounted for 12.12%, exercise for 8.11%, feeding for 6.33%, and others for 7.09%). Therefore, Ar-1 and Ar-2, respectively, as the signal-specific calls of these two contexts (Isolation and Play), had a large number of calls.

Three of nine Amur tiger call types are also uttered by captive adult Sumatran Tigers, including Chuff, Hiss, and Roar [25]. Although Growl was also identified in adult Sumatran Tigers, the Growl in our study was different from that in Rose et al.’s study [25] which had no rhythmic pulsation. The definition herein used for this call type was based on Volodina [36] and Stanton [46]: Growl was a low-pitched, rumbling sound consisting of numerous short pulses with a long duration. Comparing the spectrograms and audios of Growl from Rose et al.’s study [25] and the calls of our study, we found that the calls of Haer were similar to those of Growl. A short harsh and repeated call named ‘coughing snarl’, which was used when attacking [24], was similar to Haer in our study, which was similar to coughing and was also used to show offensive behavior toward a conspecific.

In the hierarchical cluster analysis and DFA, the samples of eee and Growl were always classified together, consistent with aural and visual sense. Both are calls with rhythmic pulsation. However, the calls of Growl were relatively rare with lower fundamental frequency and lower three quartiles frequency, and it was mostly used in the offensive context (87% in Offensive-to-Human, 12% in Offensive-to-Conspecific), while the calls of eee were mostly used in the Play context (88%), which occurred when a cub felt uncomfortable in a playing situation. The samples of Ar-2, Er, Haer, and Roar were also assessed together in the quantitative analysis; however, unlike eee and Growl, they are not consistent with aural and visual sense. Spectrograms showed that their power was mostly concentrated at low frequencies, but the spectrograms of Ar-2 and Er had clear harmonics (Fig. 2B–C). For aural sense, the calls of Haer and Roar exhibited higher power and shorter duration than the calls of Ar-2 and Er. Ar-2 vocalizations were a tonal sound similar to the “a” vowel, while Er vocalizations were also a tonal sound, but sounded like the “e” vowel. Roar and Haer sounded alike, but Haer usually occurred repeatedly in quick succession, while Roar was usually given alone or with a long interval. Regarding the behavioral context, the use of Haer was different from that of Ar-2, Er, and Roar (Table 4). Although the Ar-2, Er, and Roar vocalizations were often given in the Play context, Roar vocalizations were used to show stronger unwillingness to join in playing, sometimes even occurring with some threatening actions. Roar vocalizations of tiger cubs are powerful, harsh, and alarming, like the roar of an adult tiger [25]. Haer vocalizations were short and harsh, but less powerful than Roar and usually uttered continuously. Regarding the behavioral context, Haer was usually used to express offensive to conspecifics. It is possible that the acoustic parameters could not reflect the characteristics of the calls very well so that they could not be classified precisely in the quantitative analyses.

Meow (Mew/Meow) is the most well-known felid call [32, 47]. In our study, Meow was not recorded in Amur tigers. However, according to several other studies [24, 25], Meow has been recorded in adult tigers. In Peters [32], Mew calls were found in tiger cubs, but the author did not indicate from which tiger subspecies the calls were. During this study period, we found that Meow does exist in adult Amur tigers. Therefore, there may be vocal repertoire changes in the ontogeny of Amur tigers. More studies on the vocalization of adult Amur tigers and the comparison of vocalizations between adults and cubs need to be carried out to confirm this assumption.

Contextual use of call types

Based on previous observations, we found that eyesight can influence the behavior of Amur tiger cubs. When a newborn tiger cub without good eyesight is separated from its mother, it behaves anxiously, constantly crawling and uttering a loud sound, even with recognizes its littermates. However, when the eyesight of a tiger cub improves during the ontogeny, it realizes its littermates seemingly, and it behaves anxiously only when it is separated from both its mother and littermates. Therefore, the definition of the isolation context considered the eyesight of tiger cubs. The cubs uttered six call types (Ar-1, Ar-2, Chuff, eee, Er, Hiss), with Ar-1 (1658/2556) as the major call in this situation. Ar-1 is the context-specific call and signal-specific call of the isolation context. We, therefore, deemed Ar-1 given in the isolation context as the isolation call of Amur tiger cubs. Comparing the spectrograms of isolation calls between domestic cat kittens [8, 33] and Amur tiger cubs, we found that they were similar and both very intense and tonal in quality with a number of clear resonant frequency components. However, according to Moelk [34] and Brown et al. [33], the isolated call of domestic kittens was a complex vowel call (“mi-a:ou”), that is similar to Meow, while the isolated call of Amur tiger cubs (Ar-1) sounds like a monosyllabic “a”, different from kittens.

Haskins [48] found that kitten vocalization (from the isolation context) could prompt maternal behavior. However, we have no explicit evidence to show that the isolation call of Amur tiger cubs can influence maternal behavior. During the study, we noted that the tiger cubs would be able to emit isolation calls at a few hours after birth. From the observation of a female tiger and her babies, we found that the maternal-reared tiger cubs would not utter isolation calls until their mother left for a period of time. When keepers hold the tiger cub and make a daily check, they would be more likely to utter intense isolation calls. This situation is similar to that of kittens that were confronted with a high-arousal condition (experimenters grasped the kitten and lifted it off the ground) [8]. It was found that the kittens’ isolation calls varied their acoustic parameters between low and high-arousal conditions [8]. In our study, we did not consider the effect of arousal conditions in data collection because of the difficulty in identifying the different arousal conditions.

Three call types, Growl, Haer, and Hiss, were usually used in the offensive context by Amur tiger cubs. However, Haer was commonly used in the Offensive-to-Conspecific context, whereas Growl and Hiss were commonly used in the Offensive-to-Human context. In our study, the Offensive-to-Conspecific context always occurred during feeding. Thus, we are not sure if Haer is a specific call type for Offensive-to-Conspecific or for food conflicts. During the study, clash during non-feeding time was extremely rare. We suggest that this is caused by the captivity circumstance and the lack of territory consciousness in tiger cubs. Chuff was the signal-specific call of both Conspecific-Contact and Human-Contact contexts. It seems that Amur tiger cubs used the same vocal signal to show their amity to the other tigers and humans.

In the Play and Suckled context, the Amur tiger cubs who bit or sucked others would rarely utter calls. The calls (Ar-2, eee, and Roar) from these two contexts were all given by the cubs who were sucked or bitten to express uncomfortable feelings. It has been hypothesized that the playing of kittens is a way to practice predation [49]. According to the feeders, a cub was killed by another cub or cubs during play. Therefore, we suggest that the calls may be a vital indicator to ask for help from the mother.

Conclusion

Our study identified nine call types of Amur tiger cubs (Ar-1, Ar-2, Er, eee, Chuff, Growl, Hiss, Haer, and Roar) and their contextual use. We assigned Ar-1 from the isolation context as isolation calls of Amur tiger cubs. Our study provides advanced knowledge on the vocalization of Amur tigers. This contributes to further research on the vocal communication system of Amur tigers.

Materials and methods

Site and subjects

This study was conducted at the Amur Tiger Park, Harbin, Heilongjiang, China. The park was established in 1996 to protect the Amur tiger population. All the tiger cubs in our study were born in captivity, including hand-reared cubs and maternal-reared cubs. In total, 65 tiger cubs (21 males, 31 females, and 13 unknown) aged from 0 month to 10 months old were our subjects, including 41 hand-reared cubs and 24 maternal-reared cubs. All tigers at the Amur Tiger Park are managed in accordance with the Technical Code of Feeding and Management for Wild Animals-Amur Tiger (LY/T 2199-2013, State Forestry Administration 2013).

The tiger cubs at the Amur Tiger Park had two living environments; one was the breeding bed (about 1 m2) for the younger hand-reared cubs (under 2 months old; each litter lived in one bed); the other was the indoor enclosure (minimum of 16 m2) paired with an adjoining outdoor enclosure (minimum of 16 m2) for the older cubs (> 2 months old; hand-reared and maternal-reared cubs lived together). The breeding beds were wooden and covered with absorbent paper. The maternal-reared cubs younger than 2 months of age were usually housed in indoor enclosures with their mothers. When they were temporarily separated from their mothers, they were also kept in the breeding bed.

Call collection

All audio recordings were obtained from May 2017 to February 2018. The call collector was isolated from the tiger cubs and had zero contact with animals. Individuals were recognized and distinguished based on their stripe patterns. Calls were recorded by random sampling during routine procedures (9:00–16:00 h) [50]. The recording equipment used was a Zoom-H5 handy recorder combined with a Zoom SSH-6 stereo shotgun microphone (Zoom Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). All recordings were made at a 44.1 kHz sampling rate and 24-bit depth as uncompressed.wav files. Distance to the cubs varied from 0.5 to 5 m. Each recording lasted 0.1 to 10 min.

According to the preliminary experiment and reference researches [37, 51], six behavioral contexts in which calls were produced were preset: (1) Isolation (when a newborn cub without good eyesight was separated from its mother, or when a cub with good eyesight was separated from its mother and littermates; e.g., the cub was crawling or walking around, frequently emitting long distance calls and looking around until it was tired or met its mother, littermates or breeders, or received food from breeders. The cubs opened their eyes at 21 ~ 25 days old.); (2) Offensive-to-Human (when a cub threatened or attacked a human, e.g., by crouching, showing bare teeth, pushing ears back, and rushing at human with raised paw and slapping); (3) Offensive-to-Conspecific (when a cub threatened or attacked a conspecific, e.g., by crouching, showing bare teeth, pushing ears back, rushing at a conspecific with raising paw and slapping); (4) Conspecific-Contact (friendly greeting with conspecific, e.g., by using head or nose to touch conspecific’s face); (5) Human-Contact (friendly greeting using head or nose through the net in front of a human, while walking to and fro); and (6) Play (friendly playing with conspecifics, e.g., chasing, pouncing, gently slapping and biting other cubs or being slapped and bit by other cubs). Whenever a call was recorded, we simultaneously labeled the context of production. All recordings of maternal-reared cubs were obtained when they were separated from their mothers for additional feeding or routine health checks. Therefore, no maternal-offspring interaction behavior was recorded in our study. Moreover, any weight data available for the tiger cubs were collected at the same time.

Acoustic analyses

Calls with a high sound to noise ratio that did not overlap with other sounds were identified and analyzed using Raven Pro 1.5 (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY). To create the spectrograms, a discrete Fourier transform with an FFT size of 2048, Hann window of 1024 samples, and overlap of 50% was performed. Prior to quantitative analyses, calls were initially classified into different types based on visual and auditory differences (Ar, Er, eee, Chuff, Growl, Hiss, Haer, and Roar).

Eleven acoustic parameters were measured for each call. Duration was measured manually by selecting the entire visible portion of the call in the spectrogram window. The frequency at maximum energy (also named as peak frequency, MaxFreq) and the three quartiles (Freq25, Freq50, and Freq75), covering 25, 50, and 75% of the entire call energy, were measured automatically. The lag from the call’s start time to when maximum power first occurred (RiseTime) and the interval from Freq75 to Freq25 (interquartile range bandwidth, IQRBW) were also measured. The peak frequency contour of the fundamental frequency was used to calculate the mean (F0), standard deviation (SDF0), maximum (MaxF0), and minimum (MinF0) of the fundamental frequency band. For the calls with rhythmic pulsation, the pulse number (PulseNum) by the Standard Marker cursor was additionally counted in the spectrogram window, and the pulse rate (PulseRate) was measured.

Statistical analyses

To determine if the acoustic parameters supported the prior classification, an agglomerative hierarchical cluster analysis was performed using a Euclidean distance matrix and Ward’s linkage, combined with a principal component analysis (PCA) for all samples. Considering that large value differences among variables can influence the variability and compromise the explanatory value of PCA, scaled unit variance was used to stabilize the variance. Then, a discriminant function analysis (DFA) was used to calculate the probability of the assignment of calls to the correct types. In DFA, the call type was used as a grouping variable, and the PC score was used as an independent variable. In order to test the result of the discriminant analysis, the “leave-one-out” classification (cross-validation) was used to distinguish one call from another. A binomial test was performed to investigate whether the values of correct assignment of calls were significantly higher than those expected by chance.

Following previous research [37, 51], for the analysis of the relationships between call type and behavioral context, a call type was classified as context-specific if it was produced in the same behavioral context in more than 65% of the cases. Because multiple calls can co-occur in the same behavioral context, a standard of 65% of occurrence was used to determine whether this was the primary call type for this context (signal-specific call). The main call type in the isolation context was determined as the isolation call of Amur tiger cubs (the context-specific call and signal-specific call in the isolation context).

Statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS software (SPSS, Inc., USA) and R version 3.5 [52]; tests were two-tailed, and significance levels were set at 0.05.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article is included within the supplementary information file (Additional files: Data S1).

Abbreviations

MaxFreq:

The frequency at maximum energy

Freq25:

The frequency at 25% energy

Freq50:

The frequency at 50% energy

Freq75:

The frequency at 75% energy

IQRBW:

Interquartile range bandwidth

F0:

The mean of fundamental frequency contour

SDF0:

The standard deviation of fundamental frequency contour

MaxF0:

The maximum of fundamental frequency contour

MinF0:

The minimum of fundamental frequency contour

PulseNum:

The pulse number

PulseRate:

The pulse rate

PCA:

Principal component analysis

PC:

Principle component

DFA:

Discriminant function analysis

BS:

Suckled

CC:

Conspecific-Contact

HC:

Human-Contact

IS:

Isolation

PL:

Play

OH:

Offensive-to-Human

OC:

Offensive-to-Conspecific

References

  1. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Bergman T, Fischer J, Zuberbühler K, Hammerschmidt K. The central importance of information in studies of animal communication. Anim Behav. 2010;80(1):3–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain Lang. 2010;115(1):92–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Manser MB. Referents and semantics in animal vocalizations. In: Bee MA, Miller CT, editors. Psychological mechanisms in animal communication. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2016. p. 223–49.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  4. Laiolo P. The emerging significance of bioacoustics in animal species conservation. Biol Conserv. 2010;143(7):1635–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Konerding WS, Zimmermann E, Bleich E, Hedrich H-J, Scheumann M. Female cats, but not males, adjust responsiveness to arousal in the voice of kittens. BMC Evol Biol. 2016;16(1):157.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Rodríguez-Gironés MA. Sibling competition stabilizes signalling resolution models of parent-offspring conflict. Proc Biol Sci. 1999;266(1436):2399–402.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Johnstone RA, Roulin A. Sibling negotiation. Behav Ecol. 2003;14(6):780–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Scheumann M, Roser A-E, Konerding W, Bleich E, Hedrich H-J, Zimmermann E. Vocal correlates of sender-identity and arousal in the isolation calls of domestic kitten (Felis silvestris catus). Front Zool. 2012;9(1):36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9.  Lingle S, Wyman MT, Kotrba R, Teichroeb LJ, Romanow CA. What makes a cry a cry? A review of infant distress vocalizations. Current Zoology 2012;58(5):698-726.

  10. Lingle S, Riede T, Natural history, Mark AM. Deer mothers are sensitive to infant distress vocalizations of diverse mammalian species. Am Nat. 2014;184(4):510–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Miller DE, Conover MR. Chick vocal patterns and non-vocal stimulation as factors instigating parental feeding behaviour in the ring-billed gull. Anim Behav. 1983;31(1):145–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Rodriguez-Girones AM. Sibling competition stabilizes signalling resolution models of parent-offspring conflict. Proc Biol Sci. 1999;266(1436):2399–402.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Haskins R. A causal analysis of kitten vocalization: An observational and experimental study. Anim Behav. 1979;27:726–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Schleich CE, Busch C. Juvenile vocalizations ofCtenomys talarum(Rodentia: Octodontidae). Acta Theriol. 2002;47(1):25–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Kitchener AC, Yamaguchi N. Chapter 4 - what is a Tiger? Biogeography, morphology, and taxonomy. In: Tilson R, Nyhus PJ, editors. Tigers of the world (second edition). Boston: William Andrew Publishing; 2010. p. 53–84.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  16. Panthera tigris ssp. altaica. The IUCN red list of threatened species 2011: e.T15956A5333650 [https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2011-2.RLTS.T15956A5333650.en].

  17. Kawanishi K, Seidensticker J. Chapter 11 - collaboration and partnerships are essential to sustain wild Tiger populations. In: Tilson R, Nyhus PJ, editors. Tigers of the world (second edition). Boston: William Andrew Publishing; 2010. p. 175–84.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  18. Gu J, Guo Y, Stott P, Jiang G, Ma J. A comparison of reproductive parameters of female Amur tigers (Panthera tigris altaica) in the wild and captivity. Integr Zool. 2016;11(1):33–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Liu Z, Jian MA, Teng L. Behavioral differences of male and female Amur Tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) in captivity. Zool Res. 2002;23(4):284–7.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Liu X. Raising SiberianTigers in captivity in Northeast China. J Northeast Forestry Univ. 1990;1(1):72–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Zhang M, Zhang C, Liu D. Preliminary study on parturition behavior of semifree-ranging Amur tigers(Panthera tigris altaica). Acta Theriologica Sinica. 2006;26(3):255–61.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Teng L, Feng LI, Liu Z. Behavior observation of Amur tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) in captivity. J For Res. 2002;13(3):241–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Liu Z. Mating behavior of captive Amur Tigers (Panthera tigris altaica). Chin J Zool. 2003;(1):49-52.

  24. Sunquist MSF. Wild cats of the world. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press; 2002.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  25. Rose SJ, Allen D, Noble D, Clarke J. Quantitative analysis of vocalizations of captive Sumatran tigers ( Panthera tigris sumatrae ). Bioacoustics. 2017;27:1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  26. An J, Johnson MT, Walsh EJ, Joann MG, Armstrong DL. Discrimination of individual tigers (Panthera tigris) from long distance roars. J Acoust Soc Am. 2013;133(3):1762–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Peters G, Peters MK. Long-distance call evolution in the Felidae: effects of body weight, habitat, and phylogeny. Biol J Linn Soc. 2010;101(2):487–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Titze IR, Fitch WT, Hunter EJ, Fariborz A, Douglas M, Armstrong DL, et al. Vocal power and pressure-flow relationships in excised tiger larynges. J Exp Biol. 2010;213(22):3866–73.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Sarah A, Klemuk TR, Walsh EJ, Titze IR. Adapted to roar: functional morphology of tiger and lion vocal folds. PLoS One. 2011;11(6):e27029.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Weissengruber GE, Forstenpointner G, ., Peters G, Kübber-Heiss A, ., Fitch WT: Hyoid apparatus and pharynx in the lion (Panthera leo), jaguar (Panthera onca), tiger (Panthera tigris), cheetah (Acinonyxjubatus) and domestic cat (Felis silvestris f. catus). J Anat 2010, 201(3):195-209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Hast MH. The larynx of roaring and non-roaring cats. J Anat. 1989;163(4):117.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Peters G. Dominant frequency of loud mew calls of felids (Mammalia: Carnivora) decreases during ontogenetic growth. Mammal Rev. 2011;41(1):54–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Brown KA, Buchwald JS, Johnson JR, Mikolich DJ. Vocalization in the cat and kitten. Dev Psychobiol. 1978;11(6):559–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Moelk M. Vocalizing in the house-cat; a phonetic and functional study. Am J Psychol. 1944;57(2):184–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Romand R, Ehret G. Development of sound production in normal, isolated, and deafened kittens during the first postnatal months. Dev Psychobiol. 1984;17(6):629–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Volodina EV. Vocal repertoire of the cheetah Acinonyx jubatus (Carnivora, Felidae) in captivity: sound structure and their potential for estimating the state of adult animals. Zoologicheskiĭ Zhurnal. 2000;79(7):842–3.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Smirnova DS, Volodin IA, Demina TS, Volodina EV. Acoustic structure and contextual use of calls by captive male and female cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus). PLoS One. 2016;11(6):e0158546.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Ashley LP. Acoustic analysis of vocal development in a New World primate, the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). J Acoust Soc Am. 2006;3(120):1655-70.

  39. Takahashi DY, Fenley AR, Teramoto Y, Narayanan DZ, Borjon JI, Holmes P, et al. The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal production. Science. 2015;349(6249):734–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Takahashi Daniel Y, Fenley Alicia R, Ghazanfar Asif A. Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys. Philos Transact Royal Soc B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1693):20150370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Berg KS, Beissinger SR, Bradbury JW. Factors shaping the ontogeny of vocal signals in a wild parrot. J Exp Biol. 2013;216(2):338.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Mumm CAS, Knörnschild M. The vocal repertoire of adult and neonate Giant Otters (Pteronura brasiliensis). PLoS One. 2014;9(11) :e112562.

  43. Campbell P, Pasch B, Warren AL, Phelps SM. Vocal ontogeny in Neotropical singing mice (Scotinomys). PLoS One. 2014;9(12):e113628.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Klenova AV, Goncharova MV, Bragina EV, Kashentseva TA. Vocal development and voice breaking in demoiselle cranes (Anthropoides virgo). Bioacoustics. 2014;23(3):247–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hongwei Y. The research on the behavior of Amur Tiger cub in captivity. Master. Northeast Forestry University; 2006:10.

  46. Stanton LA, Sullivan MS, Fazio JM. A standardized ethogram for the felidae: a tool for behavioral researchers. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015;173:3–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Peters G, Baum L, Peters MK, Tonkin-Leyhausen B. Spectral characteristics of intense mew calls in cat species of the genus Felis (Mammalia: Carnivora: Felidae). J Ethol. 2008;27(2):221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Haskins AR. Effect of kitten vocalizations on maternal behavior. J Comp Physiol Psychol. 1977;91(4):830–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Beaver BV. Chapter 7 - feline Ingestive behavior. In: Feline Behavior (Second Edition). Saint Louis: W.B. Saunders; 2003. p. 100–26.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  50. Altmann J. Observational study of behavior: sampling. Methods. 1974;49(3-4):227.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Salmi R, Hammerschmidt K, Doran-Sheehy DM. Western Gorilla vocal repertoire and contextual use of vocalizations. Ethology. 2013;119(10):831–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. R Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2018.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank Jiahui Qiu for his support, the staff of Harbin Amur Tiger Park for collecting data, and Jingxuan Wang and Heqin Cao for processing the data.

Funding

This study was funded by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (2572017PZ14), the National Key Programme of Research and Development, Ministry of Science and Technology (2016YFC0503200) and NSFC (31872241) to Guangshun Jiang.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

XK, XX, GJ and DL designed the study; XK, DL, YC, QW, SY, XL, and MG collected the data; XK, XX and GJ analyzed the data; XK, XX, GJ, AK and NR prepared the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Xiaoying Xing or Guangshun Jiang.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures involving animals were approved and followed the animal care guidelines issued by the Laboratory Animal Welfare and Ethics Committee of Northeast Forestry University (permit number 2020020).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Data S1

. Acoustic measurements of nine call types for describing the acoustics of call types.

Additional file 2: Table S2

. Discriminant function analysis with Principal Component scores for the classification of nine call types.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kong, X., Liu, D., Kathait, A. et al. Behavioral-psychological motivations encoded in the vocal repertoire of captive Amur tiger (Panthera tigris altaica) cubs. BMC Zool 7, 2 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40850-021-00102-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40850-021-00102-9